Plato's Cave: Gestalt Examples

Gestalt Examples of Perceptual Phenomena

The Kanizsa Figure

The Kanizsa figure generates a global percept of an occluding figure from four "pac-man" figures. A single pac-man creates no such figure, and it is only the globally consistent grouping of the four pac-men that creates the illusion. Local processing therefore cannot detect the parts of this illusory figure.

The Global Precedence Effect

Psychophysical studies have shown that when presented with local / global letter patterns like these, subjects more readily recognize the global letter than the local one; i.e. their performance is faster and more accurate when the task involves reporting the global letter than the local letter.

The Glass Pattern

The Glass Pattern is created by generating a pattern of random dots, printing that pattern, then overprinting it with another copy of the pattern rotated through a small angle. This creates a percept of a globally consistent concentric arrangement for which there is no specific local evidence.

Visual Search

When the two elements on the left are combined as shown, they create a figure which is fundamentally different from the elements of which it is composed. Besides just appearing different, psychophysical studies have shown that the combined figures are perceived in parallel when hidden amongst distractors in a visual search task, whereas control features composed of the same elements are perceived serially. This suggests that the three-dimensional percept is a low-level pre-attentive pheonomenon, rather than a high-level cognitive phenomenon.

The Ponzo Illusion

In the Ponzo illusion the two vertical lines appear to have different lengths, due to the presence of the converging lines. This percept can be explained by a perspective interpretation, where the converging lines suggest that the line on the right is farther away, and a farther object that subtends the same visual angle must actually be larger, as shown in the picture on the right. It is interesting that this illusion (the one on the left) generates the illusory size disparity without generating a percept of depth, although the illusion only makes sense when considered in depth. This suggests that depth perception is not a high-level cognitive phenomenon, but a low-level preattentive one.

The Mueller / Lyer Illusion

A similar phenomenon is observed in the Mueller/Lyer illusion where the vertical lines which are of the same length appear to be of different lengths. Again this illusion makes sense in a spatial context, because the line which is made to appear farther in depth becomes perceptually larger, while the one that is made to appear closer in depth becomes smaller. Again, this depth effect is pre-attentive and unconscious, thus ruling out a cognitive explanation.

Adelson's Illusions

Adelson presents a number of interesting depth percepts. In the first one (top left) the two circled edges are locally identical, although one is perceived as a form edge with no change in reflectance, while the other appears as a reflectance edge with no form edge.

In the next illusion (top center & top right) these two figures are identical except for a small difference at the circled corner. This local difference however propagates throughout the entire figure, generating a completely different percept of depth.

The last example (bottom row) shows how three figures which are topologically identical generate completely different spatial percepts, with form edges and illuminance edges interchanging in response to subtle differences in the global configuration.

All of the above examples defy explanation by a reductionist model which builds up the global percept by assembling the local elements, because these examples show that identical local elements can lead to very different percepts, i.e. the local interpretation takes into account the global configuration.

Return to argument

Return to Steve Lehar